Atormac
Neurology India
menu-bar5 Open access journal indexed with Index Medicus
  Users online: 2702  
 Home | Login 
About Editorial board Articlesmenu-bullet NSI Publicationsmenu-bullet Search Instructions Online Submission Subscribe Videos Etcetera Contact
  Navigate Here 
 Search
 
   Next article
   Previous article 
   Table of Contents
  
 Resource Links
    Similar in PUBMED
   Search Pubmed for
   Search in Google Scholar for
    Article in PDF (68 KB)
    Citation Manager
    Access Statistics
    Reader Comments
    Email Alert *
    Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)  

 
  In this Article
   References

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed3546    
    Printed120    
    Emailed4    
    PDF Downloaded77    
    Comments [Add]    

Recommend this journal

 


 
INVITED COMMENTS
Year : 2005  |  Volume : 53  |  Issue : 2  |  Page : 165-166

Invited Comment


Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Johns Hopkins Hospital, Baltimore, MD, USA

Correspondence Address:
A E Hillis
Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Johns Hopkins Hospital, Baltimore, MD
USA
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None


Rights and PermissionsRights and Permissions



How to cite this article:
Hillis A E. Invited Comment. Neurol India 2005;53:165-6

How to cite this URL:
Hillis A E. Invited Comment. Neurol India [serial online] 2005 [cited 2019 Sep 19];53:165-6. Available from: http://www.neurologyindia.com/text.asp?2005/53/2/165/16399


Primary progressive aphasia (PPA), characterized by deterioration in language for at least 2 years before the onset of other cognitive deficits, has been widely recognized as a distinct clinical entity since Mesulam's description in 1982.[1] However, as well characterized in the article, 'Primary Progressive Aphasia: A Comparative Study of Progressive Nonfluent Aphasia and Semantic Dementia' in this issue, PPA exists in two forms. The first, progressive nonfluent aphasia (PNFA) is often compared to Broca's aphasia, since patients often have effortful articulation, agrammatic sentence production, and relatively intact comprehension. Patients with both PNFA and Broca's aphasia due to stroke frequently have more difficulty naming verbs than nouns.[2] Likewise, semantic dementia (SD) is often compared to Wernicke's aphasia, since both are characterized by fluent, grammatical, and well-articulated speech with little content, impaired comprehension, and disproportionate difficulty naming nouns relative to verbs. As demonstrated with voxel-based morphometry, the brain regions affected in PNFA generally lie within the vascular territory typically damaged in Broca's aphasia, the left inferior frontal gyrus and anterior insula.[3] Semantic dementia, like Wernicke's aphasia, often reflects atrophy in the left temporal lobe, although the abnormality is generally more anterior and inferior in SD.

However, there are important differences from the vascular syndromes of Broca's and Wernicke's aphasia. Individuals with PNFA often have relatively spared spelling of both verbs and nouns.[2] Such a pattern is extraordinarily rare in Broca's aphasia. The dissociation between written language (intact) and other aspects of communication - speech articulation, oral naming of verbs, and grammatical sentence production (which are impaired) may provide insights into brain/language relationships. For example, brain regions that are spared in PNFA but damaged in Broca's aphasia after stroke, are good candidates for the neural regions subserving written naming.

In the same vein, there are crucial differences between SD and Wernicke's aphasia. Most notably, patients with SD typically have associative agnosia - impaired access to the meaning of objects - not just the names of objects. They often use objects inappropriately, despite normal visual perception. This deficit has not been described in Wernicke's aphasia caused by unilateral stroke. Identifying areas of the brain that are dysfunction in SD but not in Wernicke's aphasia would provide clues as to the areas responsible for object meaning. To illustrate, SD is generally associated with atrophy in temporal areas that are inferior and anterior to Wernicke's area, and more bilateral. Thus, bilateral inferior and anterior temporal cortex may be crucially involved in accessing the meanings of objects and their associations.

Thus PPA and other focal dementias provide the opportunity to investigate the functions of brain regions that are not often damaged by stroke. However, progress in this domain has been limited, largely because criteria for classification are insufficiently objective and reliable to ensure that investigators worldwide classify patients the same way. For example, the classification of PNFA requires agrammatism and/or impaired articulation for some authors[3] but not others.[4] This dissonance is unsurprising, given that speech 'fluency' is a multidimensional characteristic that encompasses parameters of melody, phrase length, syntax, articulatory agility, and rate of speech. One patient might be fluent along one dimension and nonfluent along another. This problem of classification in nontrivial, since different types of PPA may have different etiologies, but such a clinicopathological relationship has been obscured by different criteria for classification across centers. Studies that carefully describe characteristics of aphasia underlying classification, such as the study in this issue,[5] represent an important first step in forming a set of criteria for distinguishing various types of PPA with high interjudge reliability. Achieving this goal is essential for future multi-center clinical trials of treatment for PPA.



 
  References Top

1.Mesulam MM. Slowly progressive aphasia without generalized dementia. Ann Neurol 1982;11:592-8.  Back to cited text no. 1  [PUBMED]  
2.Hillis AE, Oh S, Ken L. Deterioration of naming nouns versus verbs in primary progressive aphasia. Ann Neurol 2004;55:268-75.  Back to cited text no. 2  [PUBMED]  [FULLTEXT]
3.Gorno-Tempini ML, Dronkers NF, Rankin KP, Ogar JM, Phengrasamy L, Rosen HJ, et al. Cognition and anatomy in three variants of primary progressive aphasia. Annal Neurol 2004;55:335-46.  Back to cited text no. 3  [PUBMED]  [FULLTEXT]
4.Graham NL, Patterson K, Hodges JR.When more yields less: speaking and writing deficits in nonfluent progressive aphasia. Neurocase 2004;10:141-55.  Back to cited text no. 4  [PUBMED]  [FULLTEXT]
5.George A, Mathuranath PS. Primary progressive aphasia: A comparative study of progressive nonfluent aphasia and semantic dementia. Neurol India 2005;53:162-5.  Back to cited text no. 5    




 

Top
Print this article  Email this article
Previous article Next article
Online since 20th March '04
Published by Wolters Kluwer - Medknow